Loading...

Portuguese Morphophonology: A Generative-Markedness Approach

Volume 2 Nominals

by Arthur Brakel (Author)
©2022 Monographs X, 488 Pages

Summary

‘Nominals’ examines and evaluates inflectional patterns for unproblematic nouns, adjectives, pronouns and demonstratives. The section on suffixal derivation of ordinary nominals evaluates the productivity and markedness of noun and adjective forming suffixes. An entire chapter demonstrates how the suffix +sion++e+ varies in words such nação ‘nation’ and nacional ‘national’. Problematic phenomena concerning gender and number occur when specific deriving suffixes attach or fail to attach to specific nominals. Diminutive and augmentative suffixes not only convey affect, they also work as deriving morphemes. Prefixes have a major role in nominal formation. Compound nominals are either true or semi-compounds, lexical or grammatical. Each chapter provides a grammar of the phenomena examined.

Table Of Contents

  • Cover
  • Title
  • Copyright
  • About the author
  • About the book
  • This eBook can be cited
  • Table of Contents
  • 6 The Morphophonology of Number and Gender: Unproblematic Nominals
  • Introduction
  • The Morphophonological Structure of Open-Ended Nominals
  • The Morphophonological Structure of Grammatical Nominals
  • Conclusions
  • Appendix A Nominal Classes
  • Appendix B Grammatical Parameters
  • The Pronouns
  • The Parameters
  • Appendix C Rules as they Appear in Chapter 6
  • Appendix D The Grammar of Unproblematic Nominal Inflection
  • 7 Nominal Derivation: Suffixes
  • Introduction
  • Derivational Suffixes
  • The Formation of Adjectives
  • Markedness
  • Conclusions
  • Appendix A Rules as they Appear in Chapter 7
  • Appendix B The Grammar of Nominal Derviation
  • Appendix C Suffix Classes
  • 8 Allomorphy: +sion++e+
  • Introduction
  • Allomorphy: Forms and Rules
  • Conclusions
  • Appendix A Rules as they Appear in Chapter 8
  • Appendix B The Grammar of +sion++e+’s Allomorphy
  • Appendix C D’Andrade Pardal’s Analysis of Deverbal Noun Formation: Suffix +sion++e+
  • 9 The Morphophonology of Number and Gender: Problematic Nominals
  • Introduction
  • Nasal Phenomena: Deriving Suffixes and Number
  • Genderless Numberless Nominals
  • Feminine Gender and the Problematic Nominals
  • Stems Ending in /l# with a [+feminine] Reflex Ending in <a#
  • Conclusions
  • Appendix A The Rules as they Appear in Chapter 9
  • Appendix B The Grammar of Problematic Nominals’ Inflection
  • Appendix C Pluralization and the Adjective-Deriving Suffix +bil#
  • 10 Derivations with Diminutives and Augmentatives
  • Introduction
  • Diminutives
  • Augmentatives
  • Conclusions
  • Appendix A Rules as they Appear in Chapter 10
  • Appendix B The Grammar of Diminutive and Augmentative Formation
  • Rule Order
  • Sample Derivations
  • Appendix C The Intricacies of the Diminutive Suffix +ul+
  • 11 Derivation with Prefixes
  • Introduction
  • Prefixes and Productivity
  • Prefixes and Their Markedness
  • Prefixal Allomorphy
  • Conclusions
  • Appendix A Prefixes, Stems, and Roots: Meaning
  • Appendix B The Grammar of Prefixal Derivation
  • 12 Compound Nominals
  • Introduction
  • Lexical True Compounds
  • Semi-compound Nominals
  • Grammatically Formed Compounds
  • Conclusions
  • Appendix A Compound Nominals: Structures and Rules
  • Appendix B Lexical Nominals: True Compounds and Semi-compounds
  • Rules, Structures, and Derivations
  • Derivations
  • Appendix C The Grammar of Grammatical Nominals
  • References
  • Index

←x | 1→

6 The Morphophonology of Number and Gender: Unproblematic Nominals

Introduction

§6.0.0 Nominals Identified: This chapter addresses the number and gender morphology and phonology related not only to nouns but also to adjectives, articles, demonstratives, pronouns, and infinitives. They are all nominals because, by and large, they share both syntactic and morphological properties. Syntactically, almost all of them can be subjects of verbs, objects of verbs, and objects of prepositions, as Table 6.1 demonstrates:

Table 6.1 Shared Rolls—Nominals

Open-Ended Nominals

Sets

Nominals

Subject

Object of Verb

Object of Preposition

1

Noun: pinguim

os pinguins ficam

quero pinguins

de pinguins não falo

‘penguin’

the penguins stay

I want penguins

of penguins I do not speak

2

Adjective: velho

os velhos ficam

quero velhos

de velhos não falo

‘old’

the old stay

I want old (items)

I don’t speak of old (items)

3

Infinitive: falar

falar não é possível

quero falar

acabo de falar

‘to speak’

to speak isn’t possible

I want to speak

I have just spoken

←1 | 2→Grammatical Nominals

Sets

Nominals

Subject

Object of Verb

Object of Preposition

4

Def. Article: o

o que vai é meu

quero o que vai

falo de o que vai

‘the’

the one going is mine

I want the one going

I speak of the one going

5

Indef. Article: um

umas ficam

quero umas

de umas não falo

‘a(n)’

some stay

I want some

of some I don’t speak

6

Demonstrative: este

este fica

quero este

deste não falo

‘this’

this stays

I want this

of this I shall not speak

7

Pronoun:a ela

ela fica

quero ela

dela não falo

‘she/her’

she stays

I want her (=she)

of her I shall not speak

8

Object Prn.b os

***

quero-os

***

‘them’

***

I want them

***

9

Possessive: nossos

os nossos ficam

quero os nossos

dos nossos não falo

‘ours’

ours stay

I want ours

of ours I shall not speak

a This pronoun is not strictly nominal since it can occur as an object of prepositions and, in the everyday speech of most Brazilians, as a direct object.

b This object pronoun is accusative. The morphology and phonology of object pronouns includes dative (indirect object pronouns) and reflexive pronouns.

§6.0.1 Table 6.1 is divided into two sections: Open-ended Nominals and Grammatical Nominals. Sets 1, 2, and 3 (nouns, adjectives, and infinitives) are open-ended both because they are bona fide words—they may begin or end utterances—and because their inventory is open-ended. At any time, new nouns, adjectives, and infinitives may be coined, either from Portuguese sources or from words barrowed from other languages. The grammatical nominals (Sets 4–9) are paradigmatic forms with a closed inventory, and some are restricted to specific semantic and syntactic circumstances. They are essentially nominals, however.

§6.0.2 Set 1: There is no controversy as far as the sentences in set 1 of Table 6.1 are concerned since pinguim is a noun, and it is an ideal subject, verbal object, and an object of a preposition.←2 | 3→

§6.0.3 Set 2: Velho ‘old’ is incontrovertibly an adjective, and one might argue that in these sentences some noun has been deleted. In addition, it is of course true that sentences like these would only make sense in the real-world if listeners knew exactly of whom or about what (i.e. some actual noun) the speaker was talking. Still, it is the case that in Portuguese most adjectives can take the place of whatever nouns they might describe, and, once they do, they function as subjects as well as verbal and prepositional objects.1 And this is precisely one of the two reasons2 why both nouns and adjectives are nominals.

§6.0.4 Set 3: Infinitives function as nouns because they can occur as sentence subjects, direct objects, and objects of prepositions.3←3 | 4→

§6.0.5 Set 4 The examples of the definite article as nouns are problematic. One could be justified in assuming that the adjective clause que vai has been nominalized and it, rather than the definite article, is the subject of the verb é. Likewise, the same phrase (que vai) could be seen as the object of Quero and the object of the preposition de. Another alternative, which would eliminate o as subject and object, is to consider structures like o que vai as nominalized adjective clauses.

§6.0.6 Set 5: The indefinite article shares the same relationship to nouns that adjectives do. Like adjectives it can substitute for the nouns it modifies, and when it does, it too assumes the syntactic role of a noun. In its singular forms, the indefinite article, um or uma, doubles as a numeral: um moço can mean both ‘a young person’ or ‘one young person’, uma moça may indicate ‘one young female person’ or ‘a young female person’. There is no distinction between singular indefinite articles and singular numerals in Portuguese.

§6.0.7 Set 6: This demonstrative (deictic) pronoun appears in its non-feminine, non-plural form and functions as a verbal subject and object, as well as an object of a preposition. Portuguese actually has three different demonstratives, and they perform similarly to adjectives: they modify nouns, they can substitute for nouns, and they are inflected for gender and for number.

§6.0.8 Set 7: The 3rd person pronoun (in this case, ela4) functions in all varieties of Portuguese as a subject or as the object of prepositions. The verbal direct object ela has become wide-spread in Brazilian Portuguese, at least in casual speech. Speakers of European Portuguese do not use subject pronouns as direct objects, they would say Quero-a for ‘I want her’.

§6.0.9 Set 8 demonstrates the 3rd person direct object pronoun. It cannot occur as either object of a preposition or as a subject. The pronouns of Sets 7 and 8 are high valence nouns insofar as they, like infinitives, never function as adjectives. On top of that, they are never modified by adjectives without a copula: Ele é velho ‘He is old’ but *Ele velho foi comigo ‘*Old he went with me’. And whereas quero-os means ‘I want them’, quero os velhos only means ‘I want the old ones’—the ←4 | 5→direct object os is homophonous but not homologous with the definite article os. (See Table 6.18 and §6.2.1.0–.1, §6.2.5.3.0–4.2.)

§6.0.10 Set 9: This set demonstrates the use of the possessive nosso ‘our’ clearly nominalized as subject, verbal object, and as object of the preposition de. Possessive pronouns have a relatively high adjectival valence since they can be readily modified by adverbs: O carro é exclusivamente nosso ‘The car is exclusively ours’.

§6.0.11.0 The other reason for considering nouns, adjectives, definite and indefinite articles, nominative pronouns, demonstratives, possessives, 3rd person direct object pronouns, and infinitives together is that at least some of each category’s forms can reflect gender and/or number.5 Table 6.2 shows how all these forms reflect gender and number:

Table 6.2 Nominal Inflection: Gender and Number

Sets

[-feminine]

[+feminine]

[-plural]

[+plural]

[-plural]

[+plural]

1

Noun

cachorro

cachorros

cachorra

cachorras

Gloss

pup

pups

bitch pup

bitch pups

2

Adjective

cachorro novo

cachorros novos

cachorra nova

cachorras novas

Gloss

new pup

new pups

new bitch pup

new bitch pups

3

Infinitives

o dizer

os dizeres

***

***

Gloss

the saying

the sayings

***

***

4

Definite Articles

o cachorro

os cachorros

a cachorra

as cachorras

Gloss

the pup

the pups

the bitch pup

the bitch pups

5

Indefinite Article

um cachorro

uns cachorros

uma cachorra

umas cachorras

Gloss

a pup

some pups

a bitch pup

some bitch pups

6

Demonstratives

esse cachorro

esses cachorros

essa cachorra

essas cachorras

Gloss

that pup

those pups

that bitch pup

those bitch pups

7

Pronoun

ele

eles

ela

elas

Gloss

he

they

she

they (+f.)

8

Object Pronouns

quero-o

quero-os

quero-a

quero-as

Gloss

I want it

I want them

I want it (+f.)

I want them (+f.)

9

Possessives

nosso cachorro

nossos cachorros

nossa cachorra

nossas cachorras

Gloss

our pup

our pups

our bitch pup

our bitch pups

←5 | 6→§6.0.11.1 Table 6.2 shows that for all nominals standard orthography reflects plurality with a word-final –s (<z#). Beyond this, in Sets 1, 2, 4, 8, 9 the vowel –o (<o#) indicates [-feminine] gender. In the eight cases where [+feminine] gender can be indicated, –a (<a#) occurs. On the other hand, potentially word-final –e (in Sets 6 and 7) indicates [-feminine] gender, and the indefinite article (Set 5) appears to have no morpheme indicating [-feminine] gender. The 3rd person direct object ←6 | 7→pronouns in Set 8 appear to be mere sequences of regular gender ([±feminine]) and number [±number].6 Set 9: The 1st person plural form, like all possessive pronouns, reflects the number and gender of the possessed item(s) with the typical gender and number reflections.

§6.0.12.0 Tables 6.1 and 6.2 use the markedness strategies advanced in Chapter 1 and put to use there, as well as in Chapters 2, 3, 4, and 5. That is, the marked nominal cases are [+feminine] and [+plural], whereas the unmarked cases are [-feminine] and [-plural]. Such an analysis predicts: (A) In representative texts, plural nominals will be less frequent than non-plurals. (B) Non-plural nominals will be more widely interpretable than the plurals. (C) The marked gender ([+feminine]) will occur less frequently than its negation [-feminine]. (D) The marked gender will be restricted to a subset of referents. (E) The unmarked gender will refer to a more general set of referents. That is, [-feminine] may include [+feminine] referents, whereas the converse does not normally obtain: [+feminine] may not, in general, indicate non-feminine entities among particular words’ referents.7 This approach provides an economic and elegant analysis of Portuguese morphophonology.8

§6.0.12.1 Concerning the frequency of the marked cases, the Portuguese phonological dataset used in this study provides a convenient set of texts on which to test these predictions. The definite and indefinite articles and demonstrative pronouns are easily isolable in the corpora and co-occur with and modify or stand in for nouns. Table 6.3 displays their relative frequency in the dataset.

Table 6.3 Frequency Count: Marked and Unmarked Gender and Number

Gender

Number

[-feminine]

[+feminine]

[-plural]

[+plural]

Definite Articles

244

206

348

102

Indefinite Articles

38

21

58

1

Demonstrative Pronouns

16

5

20

1

Totals:

298

232

426

104

% of Totals:

56.23%

43.80%

80.37%

19.62%

Ratio: Unmarked/marked

1.29/1

4.1/1

←7 | 8→Table 6.3 bears out the prediction of text frequency. Of the 530 nominals counted in the dataset, 298 (56.23%) were [-feminine] and 426 (80.37%) were [-plural]. Beyond their textual frequency, marked and unmarked nominals’ ranges of reference are inversely related to their markedness. Two Portuguese proverbs and modifications thereof demonstrate this principle.

Table 6.4 Proverbs, Gender, and Number

Standard Proverb

Modified Proverb

[-feminine] (unmarked gender)

[+feminine] (marked gender)

1

Filho alheio come muito e chora feio

Filha alheia come muito e chora feia

‘Someone else’s child eats a lot and cries ugly’

‘Someone else’s daughter eats a lot and cries ugly’

[-plural] (unmarked number)

[+plural] (marked number)

2

(Um) lobo velho não cai em armadilha

Uns lobos velhos não caem em armadilha

‘(An) old wolf doesn’t fall into (a) trap(s)’

‘Some old wolves don’t fall into trap(s)’

§6.0.12.2.0 The two standard Portuguese proverbs occur in unmarked cases: Proverb 1 unmarked gender, Proverb 2 unmarked ←8 | 9→number. In the modified set of proverbs, the subject of Proverb 1 has changed from [-feminine] to [+feminine], and the subject of Proverb 2 has changed from singular to plural.

§6.0.12.2.1 In the case of Proverb 1, filho ‘child’ can refer to a particular child (either male or female, other than one’s own) or children in general. When the noun is filha ([+feminine]), it can refer only to a daughter—to a particular daughter, or daughters in general since it is not inflected for plurality. However, it says nothing about other people’s [-feminine] (i.e. male) offspring.

§6.0.12.2.2 In Proverb 2, the noun lobo, owing to its unmarked gender, can refer to both male and female wolves, to a particular wolf, and it can be a general statement about old wolves’ perspicacity. The [+plural] indefinite article along with form lobos says that only some subgroup of wolves does not fall into traps. Uns lobos velhos cannot refer directly to an individual, whereas the [-plural] form can denote both groups and individuals.

§6.0.12.3 Paragraphs §6.0.11.1–§6.0.12.2.2 and the tables therein confirm the status of [+plural] as the marked number and [+feminine] as the marked gender. What this means is that Portuguese has only one gender (feminine) which stands in contrast with gender’s absence or negation, which neither excludes nor identifies [+feminine] referents. Likewise, Portuguese has only one number [+plural], which is opposed by its absence or negation. The absence of plurality in nominals will be morphophonemically represented by <∅#, inflection for unmarked gender, on the other hand, will be represented by actual morphemes such as <o#, and <e#, as well as by <∅#. The morpheme <z# indicates plurality’s presence in nominals.

The Morphophonological Structure of Open-Ended Nominals

§6.1.0 It is obvious from Table 6.2 and the discussion and tables following it that the structure of most nominals is:←9 | 10→

Nominal Pattern 9101:

Now, using morphophonemic transcription, Table 6.5 shows the structure of the open-ended items in Table 6.2—all of which have monomorphemic roots.

Table 6.5 The Structure of Simple Nominals

[-feminine]

[+feminine]

[-plural]

[+plural]

[-plural]

[+plural]

1

Noun

cachorro

cachorros

cachorra

cachorras

#kašo+<o#<ø#

#kašo+<o#<z#

#kašo+<a#<∅#

#kašo+<a#<z#

2

Adjective

(cachorro) novo

novos

nova

novas

#nᴐv+<o#<∅#

#nᴐv+<o#<z#

#nᴐv+<a#<∅#

#nᴐv+<a#<z#

3

Infinitive

falar

falares

***

***

#falar#a+e+<∅#

#falar#+e+<z#

***

***

a For the sake of simplicity and space, the infinitive falar appears unanalyzed. Its formal analysis is: #stem++theme vowel#+infinitive deriver#+[-feminine]+<[±plural#, i.e. #fal++a#+r#+e+<Ø#. It has no tense-mood-aspect morpheme because it is not a verb (cf. Volume 3, Vi.0.0)

§6.1.1 Table 6.5 shows that the plural morpheme is unequivocally <z# for open-ended nominals. It also represents plurality on a significant portion of the grammatical nominals as well (see Tables 6.22, 6.24, 6.25). Indeed, nominal pluralization is largely, but not entirely, unproblematic.11 Notwithstanding the nominal plural morpheme’s ←10 | 11→unambiguous identity and the transparency of the forms in Table 6.5’s Sets 1–3, nominal pluralization impedes a phenomenon addressed in the next section: stem vowel metaphony.

§6.1.2.0 Number and Stem Vowel Metaphony: Whereas the structure illustrated in Table 6.5 obtains for all nominals as far as their number is concerned, even for simple nominals such as these, the case is not closed. There is a class of both nouns and adjectives in which the stressed stem vowel /ᴐ/ exhibits metaphony between plural and non-plural forms. That is, in the plural their tonic vowel surfaces as [ᴐ], whereas in the singular it emerges as [o]‌. Table 6.6 demonstrates this case of stem vowel metaphony.

Table 6.6 Vowel Metaphony: [-feminine] Nouns and Adjectives

Nouns

Word

Singular

Plural

Morphemic Analysis

Gloss

caroço

[kă.ˡro.sŭ]

[kă.ˡrᴐ.sŭš]

#kaˡrᴐs++o#(<z#)a

pit(s)

corvo

[ˡkor.vŭ]

[ˡkᴐr.vŭš]

#kᴐrv++o#(<z#)

crow(s)

fogo*b

[ˡfo.γŭ]

[ˡfᴐ.γŭš]

#fᴐg++o#(<z#)

fire(s)

jogo*

[ˡžo.γŭ]

[ˡžᴐ.γŭš]

#žᴐg++o#(<z#)

game(s)

osso*

[ˡo.sŭ]

[ˡᴐ.sŭš]

#ᴐs++o#(<z#)

bone(s)

ovo*

[ˡo.vŭ]

[ˡᴐ.vŭš]

#ᴐv++o#(<z#)

egg(s)

poço*

[ˡpo.sŭ]

[ˡpᴐ.sŭš]

#pᴐs++o#(<z#)

well(s)

miolo

[mi.ˡo.lŭ]

[mi.ˡᴐ.lŭš]

#miᴐl++o#(<z#)

brain(s) (matter)

tojo

[ˡto.žŭ]

[ˡtᴐ.žŭš]

#tᴐž++o#(<z#)

gorse (shrubs)

Adjectivesc

grosso*

[ˡgro.sŭ]

[ˡgrᴐ.sŭš]

#grᴐs+<o#(<z#)

gross, ill-mannered

novo*

[ˡno.vŭ]

[ˡnᴐ.vŭš]

#nᴐv+<o#(<z#)

new

famoso*d

[f.ˡmo.zŭ]

[f.ˡmᴐ.zŭš]

#fam++ᴐz+<o#(<z#)

famous

a Notice that the morphemic affinity between the gender morpheme and the roots of the nouns is ++ rather than +<. This is because the gender ([-feminine]) is intrinsic to these words’ meanings rather than a consequence of their referent’s gender. These gender morphemes are deriving morphemes rather than inflecting morphemes. On the other hand, the affinity between the adjectives and their gender morpheme appears as +< because the gender morpheme varies according to the adjectives’ referents or the nouns they modify.

b The words bearing asterisks are among the Aurélio’s 3,000 most frequent words.

c The adjectives’ [+feminine] forms also preserve the /ᴐ/ in their surface manifestations, be they singular or plural.

d Adjectives ending in –oso are by far the largest class of adjectives showing the /ᴐ/ ⇔ /o/ metaphony. The Aurélio lists 1254, 21 of which are among the 3,000 most frequent words.

←11 | 12→The words analyzed in Table 6.6 do not exhaust the category of singular/plural root vowel metaphony in Portuguese. They come from a list of 32 nouns (Cunha, 1969: 127) of this type.

§6.1.2.1 The 4th column of Table 6.6 identifies /ᴐ/, rather than /o/, as the vowel underlying both the singular and the plural forms of these words.12 This tack acknowledges that the vowel in these nouns was a short (i.e. open, lax) vowel in Latin, /ŏ/, which regularly became /ᴐ/ in Portuguese (Williams, 1962: 36).13 In addition, this identification is the cornerstone of a simple rule of stem vowel metaphony:

Rule 6.1 The Closing of Nominal Class 1 (N1) Vowels:

The environment or determinant of this rule (i.e. the material to the right of the slash) is for nominals in class N114 that are neither plural nor feminine. The non-feminine suffix must be preceded by at least one but no more than two consonants.15 The disjunction in the determinant indicates that the [-feminine] gender morpheme can be either an inflecting morpheme or a derivational one.16 The determinand, i.e. the ←12 | 13→specification to the left of the arrow indicating a transformation, more than adequately describes a stressed /ᴐ/, which in the environment of the determinant loses its lax quality ([+wide glottis]) and becomes [o]‌.

§6.1.2.2 The words of class N1 constitute a minority category in the nominal sector of the Portuguese lexicon and, quite likely owing to their frequent occurrences, native speakers learn which words undergo this transformation item by item. The adjectives ending in –oso are so common that their metaphony is easily learned. For the nouns, Cunha (127), immediately after his examples of nominal root vowel metaphony, lists another 32 nouns whose root vowel is an invariant /o/ and whose structure is similar (Table 6A.1). Identifying nouns as pertaining to Class N and identifying their root vowels as /o/ obviates their susceptibility to Rule 6.1

§6.1.2.3 The phenomenon generated by Rule 6.1 can be explained in the following manner: The presence of a [+high] back vowel [ŭ] in a syllable following one containing a non-high back vowel, especially a labial vowel such as [ᴐ], will cause the formants of the open vowel to approximate those of [o]‌.17 In the evolution of Vulgar Latin into modern Portuguese, the word-final vowel, i.e. the morpheme <o# or +o#, indicating [-feminine] gender reduced from [o] to [ŭ] earlier than it did when it was followed by the plural suffix. Words inflected for plurality, i.e. ending in <z#, end with a potentially closed syllable, and a contoidal coda such as this can impede or at least retard the rising of a tautosyllabic vowel.

§6.1.2.4 Word-final atonic vowels, especially [ŭ], undergo considerable reduction and a high degree of effacement (§3.2.1.2, §3.3.2.8, and Table 3.18b), but such is not the case when the plural suffix follows this [-feminine] morpheme. And the reason for this is that the deletion of a word-final vowel such as [ŭ] may leave a word with a somewhat aberrant syllable coda, i.e. a word-final syllable ending in other than a vowel or /n, r, l, z/ e.g. povo ‘folk’: #pᴐv++o#<∅# ➔ [ˡpo.vŭ] ➔ [ˡpov]. Loss of the vowel in a plural form would end the word with a much more aberrant consonant cluster: povos ‘peoples’: #pᴐv++o#<z# ←13 | 14→➔ [ˡpᴐ.vŭš] ➔*[ˡpᴐvš], were this gender indicating vowel deleted.18 The atonic vocalic conservatism of the potentially closed word-final syllable of plural nouns, as opposed to the volatility of the singular forms with their open syllables, contributed to the maintenance of the tonic open vowel in the preceding syllable.

§6.1.2.5 The data and analysis in §6.1.0–.2.3 account for a huge amount of the phenomena related to nominals’ grammatical number, but these data are far from closing the case on Portuguese pluralization and nominal stem vowel metaphony. Nominal gender also plays a role in nominal stem vowel metaphony, and, even in the unproblematic nominals, gender is significantly more complex.

§6.1.3.0 Simple Nominal Gender: At their most basic, nouns that refer to animate beings (human or otherwise) are inflected for gender, which can be [±feminine]. This inflection is systematic insofar as it corresponds to the actual gender of the [+animate] being particular words denote. Table 6.7 contains examples of inflected nouns:

Table 6.7 Inflected Noun Gender

Systematic Gender

[-feminine]

Gloss

[+feminine]

Gloss

1

aluno

student

aluna

female student

2

amigo

friend

amiga

female friend

3

gato

cat

gata

female cat

4

lobo

wolf

loba

female wolf

5

macaco

macaque

macaca

female macaque

6

moço

young person

moça

young woman

7

pombo

pigeon

pomba

female pigeon

8

porco

pig

porca

sowa

9

tio

uncle

tia

aunt

a In addition to meaning ‘sow’, porca is the word for ’hex nut’. Likewise, the word gata (item 4) can also indicate the third mast of a ship, loba (item 6) can indicate a cape or a perfect hand in a card game, and macaca (item 8) can indicate a cow without a calf. The idiomatic uses of these [+feminine] words justifies their entry into the Portuguese lexicon as independent words, and it justifies the assignment of idiomatic or deriving gender morphemes for the words whose meanings cannot be deduced from their parts’ sums: #pᴐrk++a#, #gat++a#, #lob++a#, and #makak++a#.

←14 | 15→Several of these items, amigo, moço, and porco, function readily as adjectives. And all but pombo are among the Aurélio’s 3,000 most frequent words. Save porco/porca, these words are Class N nouns, i.e. their [-plural] manifestations undergo no-stem vowel metaphony.

§6.1.3.1 Stem Vowel Metaphony Revisited: All N1 stems are exempted from metaphony when they occur with a [+feminine] suffix.19 Table 6.8 contains examples of this phenomenon:

Table 6.8 Vowel Metaphony: [± feminine] Nouns and Adjectivesa

Noun

Stem

[-feminine]

[+feminine]

Morphemic Analysis

Gloss

#pᴐrk+

[ˡpor.kŭ]

[ˡpᴐr.kă]

#pᴐrk+<gender#<number#

pig/sow

ADJECTIVES

#grᴐs+

[ˡgro.sŭ]

[ˡgrᴐ.să]

#grᴐs+<[αfeminine] #(<z#)

crude

#nᴐv+

[ˡno.vŭ]

[ˡnᴐ.vă]

#nᴐv+<[αfeminine] (<z#)

new

#X++ᴐz+

[f.ˡmo.zŭ]

[f.ˡmᴐ.ză]

#fam++ᴐz#+<[αfeminine]#(<z#)

famous

Deriving Gender Morphemes (Nouns)

#kᴐk+

[ˡko.kŭ]

[ˡkᴐ.kă]

#kᴐk++gender#<number#

head/spying

#korkᴐv+

[kŭr.ˡko.vŭ]

[kŭr.ˡkᴐ.vă]

#korkᴐv++gender#<number#

buck/hump

#pᴐs+

[ˡpo.sŭ]

[ˡpᴐ.să]

#pᴐs++gender#<number#

well/puddle

#pᴐzt+

[ˡpoš.tŭ]

[ˡpᴐš.tă]

#pᴐz++t++gender#<number#

job/piece

a The words in this table are porco, coxo, grosso, novo, famoso, coco/coca, corcovo/corcova, poço/poça, posto/posta.

§6.1.3.2 The metaphony displayed in Table 6.8 is most prevalent in adjectives, especially given the high-frequency (fn. 13) adjective-forming suffix +ᴐz +. The adjectives, and however many nouns of the type porco ⇔ porca there may be, undergo root vowel metaphony when their roots are inflected for [-feminine] and [-plural]. Rule 6.1 needs no ←15 | 16→alteration to account for the non-metaphony of [+feminine] inflected (or derived) N1 words.

N1 nominals’ metaphony occurs only in the context of [-feminine] items with [-plural] inflection.

§6.1.3.3 Derivational Gender: Although there are many nouns similar to those in Tables 6.7 and 6.8, there are many more Portuguese nouns like the final four of Table 6.8. These nouns bear gender morphemes, but the gender morpheme is arbitrarily attached to the nominal stem. These morphemes assign gender to the resultant nouns, but, beyond that, they can actually combine with nominal stems and derive different words using the same stem. Table 6.9 has more representative examples:

Table 6.9 Derivational Noun Gendera

[-feminine]

Analysis

Gloss

[+feminine]

Analysis

Gloss

1

maço

#mas++o#

bundle

massa

#mas++a#

dough

2

falo

#fal++o#

phallus

fala

Details

Pages
X, 488
Year
2022
ISBN (PDF)
9783034343886
ISBN (ePUB)
9783034343893
ISBN (MOBI)
9783034343909
ISBN (Softcover)
9783034338431
DOI
10.3726/b18595
Language
English
Publication date
2022 (September)
Published
Bern, Berlin, Bruxelles, New York, Oxford, Warszawa, Wien, 2022. X, 488 pp., 267 tables.

Biographical notes

Arthur Brakel (Author)

Arthur Brakel has a Ph.D. in Portuguese and Linguistics from the University of Wisconsin as well as an M.A. in Portuguese and a B.A. in Latin American studies from the University of New Mexico. He has taught Spanish, Portuguese (language and literature) and Linguistics at Kent State University, SUNY-Albany, University of Michigan, Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, and Albion College.

Previous

Title: Portuguese Morphophonology: A Generative-Markedness Approach
book preview page numper 1
book preview page numper 2
book preview page numper 3
book preview page numper 4
book preview page numper 5
book preview page numper 6
book preview page numper 7
book preview page numper 8
book preview page numper 9
book preview page numper 10
book preview page numper 11
book preview page numper 12
book preview page numper 13
book preview page numper 14
book preview page numper 15
book preview page numper 16
book preview page numper 17
book preview page numper 18
book preview page numper 19
book preview page numper 20
book preview page numper 21
book preview page numper 22
book preview page numper 23
book preview page numper 24
book preview page numper 25
book preview page numper 26
book preview page numper 27
book preview page numper 28
book preview page numper 29
book preview page numper 30
book preview page numper 31
book preview page numper 32
book preview page numper 33
book preview page numper 34
book preview page numper 35
book preview page numper 36
book preview page numper 37
book preview page numper 38
book preview page numper 39
book preview page numper 40
500 pages